Introduction
Cataract surgery is one of the most common surgeries performed in Canada and worldwide1 and it is becoming more prevalent as people in the world live longer. If left untreated, cataracts are known to have a detrimental effect on a patient’s quality of life, by lowering their independence in performing daily activities, decreasing their personal safety and increasing their likelihood of vision loss.2–4 Although permanent loss of vision from cataract surgery is rare, there are some risks of potentially sight-threatening complications. According to the literature, the overall rate of severe complications in the 1-year postoperative period following cataract surgery is 0.5%. These severe complications include infectious endophthalmitis (0.16%), retinal detachment (0.26%) and suprachoroidal haemorrhage (0.06%).5
Risk communication is an integral aspect of shared decision-making and evidence-based patient choice. In the context of making medical decisions, it has been shown that patients often overestimate their likelihood of experiencing the benefits and underestimate their likelihood of experiencing the risks.6 Previous studies have demonstrated that the perceived benefits (or magnitude of risk) of a procedure may be inflated if presented using relative risk than when using an absolute risk.7 8 Additionally, it has been proposed that the way information is framed may also play a role in decision-making. For example, a study by Bigman et al presented the effect of a human papilloma virus (HPV) prophylactic vaccines as 70% effective (positive frame) to some participants and as 30% ineffective (negative frame) to the rest.9 Findings showed that participants who received the positive frame perceived the HPV vaccine as effective and were more willing to receive it than those in the negative framed group. Similarly, another study showed that when presented with information regarding safety of a hypothetical vaccine, a positive framed information was associated with more positive attitudes towards vaccination (eg, 90% chance of no side effects vs 10% chance of side effects).10 On the other hand, it has been shown how negative framing is more effective in advising people to engage screening activities, such as breast cancer self-examination.11 12 Whether the information about the outcomes of different actions is presented as either a loss or a gain can also have varying effects.8 For instance, in the context of surgery, one could emphasise the risks and disadvantages of not getting surgery (loss framing) or the benefits and advantages of getting surgery (gain framing). Prior studies have demonstrated that loss-framed messages are expected to be more effective than gain-framed in persuading individuals to perform a health behaviour that is perceived to be risky.8 Retamero and Galesic reconciled these findings, by suggesting that gain frames can more effectively promote disease prevention behaviours, whereas loss frames can more effectively encourage disease detection behaviours.13 Other factors that may also alter patients’ risk perceptions include presenting information using numbers versus graphics, the order of presenting risks and benefits and presenting information using percentages vs numbers.7 14
Although there have been studies reporting on the capacity and quality of cataract surgery in Canada, there is little information about the determinants of cataract surgery uptake in our population.15 When considering uptake of cataract surgery, the way surgical risks and benefits are communicated must be considered because patients’ perception of surgical risks can have a substantial impact on their decision to undergo surgery. Some patients may overestimate the risks of cataract surgery, which can be a barrier to receiving surgery in a timely manner, and other patients may underestimate the risks, which can cause them to have surgery prematurely.6 A shared decision model requires patient education and clear risk–benefit communication.16–18 Family physicians, primary eye care providers and ophthalmologists all play an important role in educating patients about the risks and benefits of cataract surgery. Currently, there is no standardised or recommended way of communicating the potential benefits and risks of cataract surgery to patients, and it is usually up to each health provider’s discretion.19 This study aims to investigate whether the type of framing (positive or negative) matters when communicating risks and educating patients about cataract surgery. Recognising the impact of communication delivery on patient perception could inform clinical practice and guide patient education strategies to better ensure that patients are well-informed and empowered prior to making their decision.20